Tone and syllable structure in Jàbà
Linda Chinelo Nkamigbo
Department of Linguistics, Nnamdi Azikiwe Federal University, Awka, Nigeria
Abstract
Jàbà is a tone language. It is spoken precisely in Kaduna and some parts of Kwara states of Nigeria. The focus of this paper is twofold. Firstly, it studies tone in Jàbà. Then, it looks into the structure of Jàbà syllables. Data were collected from educated multilingual native speakers of Jàbà who are also literate in the language. The paper, then, observed that tone is phonemic and varies in the associative construction. It also found out that three level tones exist in the language namely: high, mid and low. The mid tone is unmarked. However, Jàbà permits consonant clusters at the onset; it employs both open and closed syllables.
Introduction
Jàbà is the language of the Jàbà people of Kaduna state of Nigeria. Jàbà is also the language of the Kainji people of Kwara state of Nigeria. It is spoken precisely in Kachia and Jama’a Local Government Areas of Kaduna state. It has an approximate number of one hundred thousand speakers. Jàbà belongs to Northern Jos group of the East Kainji sub branch of the Kainji language phylum of East Benue Congo family (Blench in http.// rogerblench. info/RBOP.htm). The Kwoi dialect of Jàbà is chosen for the present analysis because it is the variety spoken by my informants. This language under investigation is chosen because it is one of the neglected and highly endangered languages of Nigeria. The primary data were collected through tape-recording the speeches of some educated multilingual native speakers of Jàbà who are also literate in the language. The speakers are multilingual in Jàbà, Hausa and English.
Jàbà is a tone language because it manipulates tone contrastively. In other words, it makes use of pitch in distinguishing between identical lexical items. Pike (1948:43), in affirmation to the above notion, defines a tone language as, “A language having lexically significant, contrastive but relative pitch on each syllable”. Hyman (2007) has argued that there is no prototypical pitch accent language and that all languages that use tone phonemically should be classified as tone languages. Tones are referred to as tonemes especially when they distinguish lexical meaning.
The syllable, on the other hand, has been viewed by Mathews (1997:366) as, “A phonological unit consisting of a vowel or other unit that can be produced in isolation, either alone or accompanied by one or more less sonorous units”.
Trask (1996: 346) highlights that syllable structure is usually seen as,
The requirements and constraints which determine the shapes of possible syllables, usually formulated in terms of sequences of consonants and vowels but also in terms of onset plus rhyme, or onset plus nucleus plus coda.
The syllable has a relationship with tone in that it (syllable) is usually seen as the tone-bearing unit. Clements (1983) uses the term ‘tone-bearing unit’ (TBU) to refer to the surface domain of tone. Tone is applied to words in order to distinguish two or more words whose composition in terms of consonants and vowels is the same. In Igbo, for instance, the application of pitch variations to certain lexical item yields different meanings.
For example ‘isi’
isi ‘head’
isì ‘smell’
ìsì ‘blindness’
isī ‘to cook’
(High tones are left unmarked. Macron is used here to represent downstep)
Tone in associative construction
Tone is a phenomenon of pitch which has received much attention (See Pike (1948), Welmers (1973), Hyman (1975), Ifode (1995), Clements (2000) for example). Tone, according to Ladefoged (1993), is the distinctive pitch level of a syllable. Tone is the use of pitch in language to distinguish words. In tonal languages, tone is an integral part of a word itself.
Three level tones exist in Jàbà. They are represented in (1) below:
High ́
Mid ̄
Low ̀
In the Jàbà language, the high and low tones are marked while the mid tone is unmarked. Mid tone is phonemic in this language because it constitutes a semantic change. Also, it occurs freely in the three word positions namely Word Initial Position, Word Medial Position and Word Final Position. This is unlike the downstep in Igbo whose occurrence is predictive. They are manifested in words below:
(2)
Tone Phonetic Orthographic Gloss
mark representation representation
High ́ [zɪ́lí] zilli one
[lítsí] litsi head
Mid ̄ [nom] nom sun
[ʃɔtʃɔt] shout-shout dance
Low ̀ [sàsìt] saseit black
[ʃànʃù] shyanshu eye
It is noteworthy that in a language attesting three tones High, Mid, Low, it is more usual to leave the Mid unmarked. That is our reason for not marking the Mid tone in the above words.
We observe that in Jàbà, the tones words bear in isolation tend to change when such words are in construction with other words. This is mostly evident in numerals. For example:
(3)
Tat
[tat] ‘three’ soua-di ka tat [swàdìkátàt]
‘thirteen’
M L L H L
Noun
[naʊn]
‘four’ soua-di ka noun [swàdìkánaʊ̀n]
‘fourteen’
M L L H L
Twoh
[to] ‘five’ soua-di ka twoh [swàdìkátò]
‘fifteen’
M L L H L
Notice the change from M to L on the vowels of [swàdìkátàt], [swàdìkánaʊ̀n] and [swàdìkátò]. In the underlying structure, the vowels of [tat], [naʊn] and [to] are mid but they lose their mid tone when they associate with [swàdìká]. The tonal change is represented as follows:
(4)
Underlying
s w a d i k a t a t Segmental tier
L L H M Tonal tier
s w a d i k a t a t
L L H M L Tone deletion
Surface s w a d i k a t a t Segmental tier
L L H L Tonal tier
In the case of [swàdìkátàt], above, we observe an obligatory tone deletion. ‘X’ and the broken line show that the original M tone of the vowel [a] is obligatorily deleted in order to make a way for the L tone.
Syllable
The syllable is seen as a unit of connected speech. Although several attempts have been made towards defining the syllable, there has not been a strict definition of a syllable. Matthews (1997:366) claims that the syllable is, “a phonological unit consisting of a vowel or other unit that can be produced in isolation, either alone or accompanied by one or more less sonorous units.” Crystal (1997:164) views a syllable as, “an element of speech that acts as a unit of rhythm, consisting of a vowel, syllabic, or vowel/consonant combination.”
Urua (2000:66) adds that “the syllable provides an anchor on which a number of segmental and suprasegmental phenomena hinge.”
Eyisi (2000:161) attempts a definition of the syllable thus:
The above definitions of a syllable are only guides to using the term. There has not been a satisfactory definition of the syllable. Sommerstein (1977:199) affirms the above notion by stating:
Let us consider the English word information. It consists of four syllables – in.for.ma.tion
We can think of the information about the syllable structure as being contained within a syllable tier. Trask (1996:346) notes that syllable structure is usually seen as “the requirements and constraints which determine the shapes of possible syllables, usually formulated in terms of sequences of consonants and vowels, but also in terms of onset plus rhyme, or onset plus nucleus plus coda.” Yule (1996) also maintains that a syllable must contain a vowel (or vowel-like) sound. The basic syllable is consonant before a vowel. He points out basic elements of the syllable as:
Onset (one or more consonants)
Rhyme (consists of the vowel which is treated as the nucleus, plus any following consonant(s), treated as the coda)
The symbol σ is used to represent a syllable. The nucleus (N), rhyme is represented as (R), onset is (O). The consonants following the nucleus are grouped together as the coda (Co). So, ‘information’ could be diagrammatized as follows:
(5)
σ σ σ σ
R O R O R O R
N Co N N N Co
ɪ n f ɔ m ei ʃ Ə n
We shall present a discussion of Jàbà syllable here because the syllable is seen as the tone-bearing unit (TBU) in the language. Clements (1983) uses the term ‘tone-bearing unit’ to refer to the surface domain of tone. The phonological unit which constitutes the underlying domain of tone differs from language to language.
Basically, we are familiar with languages in which the syllable is the underlying domain of tone. In some other languages, Ukaan for instance, morphemes or other units such as the mora constitute the domain of tone.
The Jàbà syllable can be expressed in a formula thus:
(Cº ̄²) V (Cº ̄¹) or (CC) V (C)
The Jàbà syllable is schematized below:
(6)
Syl
(ONS) PEAK (CODA)
The use of broken lines in fig. 6 shows that Onset (ONS) and Coda are optional while the thick line depicts the obligatory nature of peak in every syllable of Jàbà.
Restrictions on the combinatory patterns of consonants in the Jàbà syllable structure
The consonants that occur at the onset and coda of Jàbà syllable, and the restrictions on the occurrences of the consonants would be examined here.
The combinatory constraints at the onset
Constraint 1: One consonant at the onset
All consonants but /ŋ/ can occur at the onset.
Constraint 2: Two consonants at the onset
The first consonant of the two will be either a plosive or an alveolar nasal. The following consonant will be any of an approximant, voiced bilabial plosive or voiced alveolar fricative.
For example:
/gb/ labiovelar plosive and palatal approximant /j/ as in /fɔgbjɛm/‘sky’
/k/ voiceless velar plosive and alveolar trill /r/ as in /krɪpak / ‘rock’
/t/ voiceless alveolar plosive and bilabial approximant /w/ as in /twani / ‘six’
/n/ alveolar nasal and voiced alveolar fricative /z/ as in /nzɛʤɛp/ ‘sand’
/n/ alveolar nasal and voiced bilabial plosive /b/ as in /nbuno / ‘toe’
The combinatory constraints at the coda
It should be noted here that we did not encounter such clusters as those in English here. In English, a word may have a cluster of two or more consonants at the coda. For instance,
Strength /strɛnɵ/ two consonants at the coda
Constraints /kƏnstreints/ three consonants at the coda
Jàbà is quite different from English. In Jàbà, only one consonant is found at the coda. So, we have just one constraint.
Constraint 1: One consonant at the coda
Plosives and nasals are the only consonants that are permitted at the coda. For example:
Consonant at the coda Phonemic transcription Gloss
/k/ /pak/ mountain
/g/ /ɗɔɡ/ river
/m/ /nom/ sun
/t / /jɛt / star
/p/ /titantep/ green
/ŋ/ /toŋ/ neck
/n/ /naʊn/ four
Let us present illustrations of few Jàbà syllables below:
(7)
σ σ
O R O R
N N Co
f ɔ ɡb j ɛ m
(8)
σ
O R
N Co
ɗ ɔ ɡ
Fig.7 is a representation of constraint two of the combinatory constraints at the onset while fig. 8 is that of constraint one of the combinatory constraints at the coda.
The language permits open and closed syllables. An open syllable or zero termination is one that does not allow consonants at the syllable final position (Igbo is a language that has open syllables) while a closed syllable is one that permits syllable final consonants (English is a good example of languages that have closed syllables).
open syllable closed syllable
mino /mino/ ‘cloud’ kpak /pak/ ‘mountain’
litsi /litsi/ ‘head’ kop /kop/ ‘ten’
gwanda /ɡwanda/ ‘paw-paw’ thyang /ɸɪaŋ/ ‘shoulder’
gbu /ɓu / ‘fall’ souk /sɔk/ ‘twelve’
huno /huno / ‘best’ yet /jɛt / ‘star’
Conclusion
In the light of growing interests in minority languages under the threat of endangerment, this paper has attempted an investigation into the Jàbà language of Nigeria. Having attempted a brief phonology of this language, this paper has come up with the following assumptions:
Jàbà employs tone contrastively. Three level tones are observed in the language. They are:
High ́ Mid ̄ Low ̀
While high and low tones are marked, the mid tone is unmarked. The language has both open and closed syllables. For example:
open syllable closed syllable
mino /mino/ ‘cloud’ kpak /pak/ ‘mountain’
litsi /litsi/ ‘head’ kop /kop/ ‘ten’
Consonants are permitted at the three positions namely: Syllable Initial Position (SIP), Syllable Medial Position (SMP) and Syllable Final Position (SFP) except the voiced palatal nasal /ɲ/ and the voiced labialized velar nasal /ŋʷ/. They do not occur at SFP. Vowels are restricted in occurrence. They are not allowed in SIP. A good look at the words presented in this paper would confirm that. The syllable is the tone-bearing unit.
References
Blench, R.M. An Atlas of Nigerian Languages. Third edition. Retrieved February 23, 2010, from http:// www.rogerblench.info/RBOP.htm.
Clements, G.N. (1983). Some Parameters of Variation in Tone Languages. A Paper Presented at the Conference on Hierarchy and Constituency in Phonology held at the University of Massachusetts, Amherst.
Clements, G.N. (2000). Phonology. In Heine, B. and D. Nurse (Eds.) African Languages: An Introduction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Pp. 123-160.
Crystal, D. (1997). The Cambridge Encyclopedia of Language. Second Edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Eyisi, J. (2003). The Syllable Structure of English. In Ndimele, O.M. (Ed.) Four Decades in the Study of Languages and Linguistics in Nigeria: A Festschrift for Kay Williamson. Aba: NINLAN. Pp. 248-258.
Hyman, L.M. (1975). Phonology: Theory and Analysis. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Hyman, L. (2007). There is no Pitch-Accent Prototype. A Paper Presented at the 2007 Linguistic Society of America Meeting held at Anaheim, C.A.
Ifode, S.O. (1995). Basic Phonetics. Port Harcourt: Sunray Publications.
Ladefoged, P. (1975). A Course in Phonetics. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich Publishers.
Matthews, P.H. (1997). Oxford Concise Dictionary of Linguistics. Reading: Cox and Wyman Ltd.
Pike, K. (1948). Tone Languages: A Technique for Determining the Number and Type of Pitch Contrasts in a Language with Studies in Tonemic Substitution and Fusion. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Publications.
Sommerstein. A.H. (1977). Modern Phonology. London Edward Arnold Publishers Ltd.
Trask, R.L. (1996). A Dictionary of Phonetics and Phonology. New York: Routledge.
Urua, E.E. (2000). Ibibio Phonetics and Phonology. Cape Town: Centre for Advanced Studies of African Society.
Welmers, W.E. (1973). African Language Structures. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Yule, G. (1996). The Study of Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.